BOOK REVIEW: Vanessa L. Ryan, Thinking Without Thinking in the Victorian Novel (Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press, 2012)
———————————————
Science and (the study of) literature are growing closer together.
From Stanford's Literary Lab and a recent New York Times piece on the Digital Humanities to reading Austen in an MRI machine and so-called "Literary Darwinism," there's both controversy and a certain cache (and maybe even a little cash) in bringing scientific techniques and the study of literature closer together.
This is your brain on Austen (http://news.stanford.edu/news/2012/september/images/mri_reading_news.jpg) |
Work on the interplay between science and literature has been dominated by scholars of the Victorian novel. Gillian Beer, George Levine, Nicholas Dames, Judith Ryan – all are Victorianists who put literature into dialogue with contemporaneous scientific ideas ranging from natural history to experimental psychology.
Now, with Thinking Without Thinking in the Victorian Novel—just out from Johns Hopkins University Press—Vanessa Ryan adds a new voice, and a new emphasis on the (pre-Freudian) unconscious, to the mix.
Source: http://payload56.cargocollective.com/1/4/138891/3422143/thinkingwithoutboth.jpg |
Of course, part of the reason it's scholars like Ryan examining the tangled literary-scientific web of the Victorian period—and not, or at least not nearly as much, historians—is that criticism takes training. Ryan's attention to style and form has no parallel in books by historians (of science). Sure, historians will gripe that history takes training, too, and that Ryan plays fast and loose in certain places. I, for one, found the claim that the "new psychology" was "anxious to embrace opposed and less reputable opinions" (41) a little confusing.
But overall, I think the critics clearly win the day in the literary-historical treatment of the relationship between science and literature—a state of affairs that wouldn't have been obvious a quarter-century ago. With the rise of "New Historicism" and the impact of the "Linguistic Turn" in the 1980s, critics and (intellectual) historians crossed paths on the terrain of language and meaning. Today, literary history is going strong, while (intellectual) historians who mention tropes—or read novels—are few and far between.
Source: http://ianhesketh.files.wordpress.com/2011/02/history-and-theory1.jpg |
As Ryan sees it, Sully's essay on Eliot "is an early form of the current interest in 'what novels do,' as opposed to 'what a novel is.'" Novelists like George Eliot are interesting, as both Sully and Ryan see it, "not just for the way in which they narrate the mind but also for the way they seek to engage the psychology of the reader" (165).
We're not far from Leah Price's How to Do Things with Books in Victorian Britain, and indeed much of the sharp edge of historicism as it's currently practiced in English departments is something historians of science (and pragmatists) can get behind. After all, we moved beyond demarcation to sensible effects ourselves—and, insofar as we're now returning to the demarcation problem, we've done so in terms of the stakes and sensible effects of "what a science is."
Another way to cast Ryan's accomplishment—one to which she seems amenable herself—would be to say that she has historicized a hundred-year-old version of the science-literature hybrid with which I began this post. Today, (some) scholars are asking what novels do by scanning readers' brains and/or telling evolutionary just-so stories; this is, in a sense, the analogue of Sully's reading of Eliot in terms of the prevailing psychological theories of his own day.
Let me conclude my thoughts on Ryan's excellent book with something that caught my eye the moment it arrived. Ryan's title rang a bell and I wasn't sure why—until I Googled it. In this day and age, I have to assume that books don't share (parts of) titles with international bestsellers by chance. As I suggested in a previous post, books on brains have fared well in the market defined by Malcolm Gladwell.
Source: http://www.gladwell.com/media/large/mtg_bw.jpg |
But I think we might go even further. When Ryan takes seriously the interaction between psychological theory, literary criticism, and Victorian novels, she shows how all three mutually built and defined a shared vocabulary of method with respect to the mind and its potential. Both science and fiction were imagined as "experimental," different means of what Darwin once described as "attacking the citadel."
The popularity of authors like Gladwell and Gilbert, the funding of major initiatives like Europe's Human Brain Project, and the ubiquity of fMRI suggest that mental theory's in a moment of foment. That prominence can help explain the attention scholars and historian are paying to the history of mental science, but that work can help us reflect back on our present obsessions in turn.
We might keep an eye out, as Ryan does, for Victorian convergences that look like what we're seeing today, asking questions about the motives that produced them and the vocabularies that resulted. But we might also ask Victorian questions of our own time. Specifically, how might the emergence of "neurocriticism" be impacting—negatively or positively, for better or for worse—the practices of literary production today?
———————————————
*In fact, an earlier version of this chapter of Ryan's book was published in the Journal of the HIstory of Ideas in 2009.
Note: Only a member of this blog may post a comment.